Hướng Dẫn Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 2022

Mẹo về Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 Mới Nhất

Pro đang tìm kiếm từ khóa Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 được Cập Nhật vào lúc : 2022-12-16 13:33:00 . Với phương châm chia sẻ Kinh Nghiệm Hướng dẫn trong nội dung bài viết một cách Chi Tiết 2022. Nếu sau khi đọc Post vẫn ko hiểu thì hoàn toàn có thể lại Comment ở cuối bài để Admin lý giải và hướng dẫn lại nha.

Mẹo về Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022

Pro đang tìm kiếm từ khóa Top-down processing Neuroscience được Update vào lúc : 2022-12-16 13:33:13 . Với phương châm chia sẻ Mẹo về trong nội dung nội dung bài viết một cách Chi Tiết 2022. Nếu sau khi đọc tài liệu vẫn ko hiểu thì hoàn toàn hoàn toàn có thể lại Comments ở cuối bài để Tác giả lý giải và hướng dẫn lại nha.

Major issues:

1) The empirical and conceptual advances made in the current study need to be more clearly articulated with respect to previous work. It has been known for a while that the OFA responds an earlier latency than the FFA (e.g., Liu et al., 2002), and that certain stimulus manipulations, such as face inversion and contrast reversal, lead to delayed responses to faces (Bentin et al., 1996; Rossion et al., 2000; Rossion et al., 2012). Previous fMRI work has shown that difficult to perceive Mooney faces can lead to response delays on the order of several seconds (McKeeff and Tong, 2007). More recent techniques have allowed research groups to provide more refined estimates of the timing of neural responses, such as the fusion of fMRI-MEG analyzed using representational similarity analysis (e.g., Cichy et al., 2014). Periodic visual stimulation has also been used to characterize the timing of neural responses obtained with EEG/MEG by several research groups (e.g., Rossion et al., 2012, 2014; Norcia et al., 2015), and this approach has been successfully applied to characterize top-down effects of feedback during face processing (e.g., Baldauf and Desimone, 2014).

We appreciate and agree with this suggestion. The dynamics of face induced neural activation in FFA and OFA has been studied for a long time with various techniques. However, previous results are inconsistent and individually often lack either the spatial (e.g., sensor level EEG/MEG analysis) or temporal precision (e.g., fMRI data). Our results with combined fMRI and MEG measures, provide detailed and novel timing information of the core face network. For example, the relatively large temporal gap between the right anterior and posterior FFA was not reported in previous studies. Furthermore, our results showed that the temporal relationships between OFA and FFA are dependent on the internal facial features as well the context of visual input, which helps to understand how bottom-up and top-down processing together contribute to face perception.

Many previous studies used the N170/M170 component as the index of face processing in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex, however, the delayed N170/M170 response caused by certain stimulus manipulations (eg: face inversion, Mooney transformation) represents a relatively crude measure of face processing because the difficulty in attributing the sources of the delay. On the other hand, fMRI measures alone showing delayed FFA response to Mooney faces that was initially not recognized as faces simply reflect the time it took subjects to recognize difficult Mooney faces, rather than the real-time dynamics of Mooney face processing. In contrast, our results showed that when the face features were confounded with other shadows, the top-down rpFFA to rOFA projection became more dominated.

In the revised manuscript, we discussed the different techniques used to investigate the timing of face responses and the top-down modulation in face processing reported in previous studies (Discussion section paragraph three to five).

2) Also, what is lacking significantly is the role of pSTS. We know pSTS is mostly involved in the analysis of facial muscle articulations (also called action units, AUs) and the interpretation of facial expressions and emotion, see Srinivasan et al., 2022, and Martinez, 2022. Also relevant is the role of low-level image features (Weibert et al., 2022), which is also missing from the Discussion; and, the role of color perception (Yip and Sinha, 2002; Benitez-Quiroz et al., 2022).

The temporal responses of bilateral pSTS are broader (multi-peaked) and showed lower signal-to-noise than the ventral face-selective areas (Figure 2 and Figure 2figure supplement 1). To increase our confidence about the pSTS time course, we analyzed the temporal responses of bilateral pSTS evoked by normal faces based on the additional data (Experiment 2), and the time courses basically remained the same as the previous ones (regardless of the task and face familiarity). We have added more discussion about the role of pSTS and its dynamics, especially in relation to the processing of facial expression, muscle articulations and motion.

We also thank the reviewer for reminding us about the role of low-level features including color, and have added more discussion about their role in face processing.

3) Another point that needs further discussion is the role of internal versus external face features (Sinha et al., 2006), and context (Sinha, Science 2004; Martinez, 2022). These discussions are essential to frame the results of the present paper within existing models of face perception.

We agree that it is important to understand the role of internal versus external face features. Since we were going to obtain more experimental data during the revision, we made the efforts to performed additional MEG experiments to specifically investigate the role of internal versus external face features and context (see #4 below). We have also added more discussion about them.

4) The conclusions of the study rest on the data from a single experiment, and further investigation of the putative effects of top-down feedback and predictive coding are not provided. A follow-up experiment that both replicates and extends the current findings would help strengthen the study.

We thank the editor and reviewer for pushing us to perform a follow-up experiment. We did not just one but three follow-up experiments (one replication and two extensions), which indeed replicated and significantly extended the findings reported in the original version.

We collected more data for Experiment 2 (normal unfamiliar face vs Mooney face) to confirm the previous results and performed two additional experiments to extend our findings. The replication data and the new experiments are reported in the revised manuscript.

Replication: we collected data from 15 additional subjects using normal faces and Mooney faces. The results were consistent with previous ones with enhanced statistical power (see Results).

Extension 1: To further study the role of internal (eyes, nose, mouth) versus external (hair, chin, face outline) face features, we presented distorted face images (explicit internal facial features available but spatially misarranged without changing face contour) to subjects and analyzed data as before. Consistent with our hypothesis, the clear face components (even though misarranged) evoked strong responses in rOFA, without clear evidence of a late signal corresponding to prediction error, indicating that spatial configuration of internal face features was not a prominent part of the prediction error from rFFA to rOFA. In this case, the processing sequence for the distorted faces would be similar to that elicited by normal face.

Extension 2: In a new experiment, we also investigated the role of context in face processing by presenting three types of stimuli to subjects: (i) images of highly degraded faces with contextual body toàn thân toàn thân cues which imply the presence of faces, (ii) images of degraded faces and body toàn thân toàn thân cues arranged in an incorrect configuration and thus do not imply the presence of faces, (iii) images of objects. Results showed that rOFA, rpFFA and raFFA are activated almost simultaneously a late stage, implying a parallel contextual modulation of the core faceprocessing network. This result further emphasize the importance of internal face features in driving the sequential OFA to FFA processing, and help our understanding of the dynamics of contextual modulation in face perception.

5) The reported effects pass statistical significance but not by a large margin. Moreover, there can be concerns that MEG data varies considerably across participants and can lead to heterogeneity of variance, especially across time points. Shuffling of the data with randomized labels would provide a more rigorous approach to statistical analysis.

As described in #4 above, we collected data from additional 15 subjects for the Mooney face experiment (normal unfamiliar faces vs. Mooney faces). Combined with previous data, nonparametric permutation tests were performed to check the significance level of observed time difference between rOFA and rpFFA. The results are consistent with previous ones with enhanced statistical power (see Results).

Reviewer #1:

[] The results are clear-cut and the paper is in general well-written. I believe the present study, if in the end published, would be of interests to a broad readership including psychologists and neuroscientists. I only have a few comments that I wish the authors to address:

1) While recognizing faces from Mooney images would certainly rely heavily on top-down mechanisms, it is hard to rule out the involvement of top-down mechanisms when processing normal face pictures. Intuitively, for example, processing familiar faces would involve more top-down experience driven activity than processing unfamiliar faces. However, the present results seem to suggest no significant differences between processing famous and unfamiliar faces. How come?

This is a very valid point. This comment helped us to clarify that the difference between processing Mooney images and normal faces are not absolute. While the top-down mechanisms are more dominant in the case of Mooney faces, it is certainly also involved, but to a less degree, in the processing of normal faces. With regard to the processing of familiar vs. unfamiliar faces, our data show that there was little difference between them. It is likely that familiarity plays a more important role in the more anterior and medial regions of the temporal cortex. We clarified our writings and discussed this issue in the revised manuscript.

2) The Discussion somewhat overlooks effects potentially driven by different tasks. As far as I understand, subjects performed different tasks for the Mooney face experiment and normal face versus object picture experiments.

We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. Yes, category task (face or not) was used in normal (familiar or unfamiliar) faces vs objects experiment, and one-back task was used in normal unfamiliar faces vs Mooney faces experiment. We had the opportunity to check the effects of task using the unfamiliar faces, since the same stimuli were used in the category task and the one-back task. Results show that there was no significant task effect in the timing of activation of the core face areas. We added more description about the different tasks used in the Materials and methods section and also added some discussion in the Discussion section.

3) Given studies on the functional role of left FFA (e.g., Meng et al., 2012; Bi et al., 2014; Goold and Meng, 2022), I would be greatly interested in Results and Discussions regarding what the present data could reveal about dynamic relations between the left and right face processing core networks.

We agree that the dynamic relations between the left and right face networks are interesting. Our results include data from both left and right face networks, though it was not feasible to further separate the left FFA into the anterior and posterior regions. We have added more discussion about the differences between left and right face processing core networks.

4) Some justification would be helpful for using sliding time windows of 50 ms. One possibility is to add power spectrum analysis. In any cases, power spectrum analysis might be helpful for revealing further fine-scale temporal dynamics of brain responses.

The 50 ms time window was selected based on previous study (Ashrafulla et al., 2013), which is a compromise in balancing the temporal precision and reliability of causality analysis. In other words, there is a trade-off between temporal resolution (shorter is better) and accuracy of model fit (longer is better) when considering the size of time window. In addition, we did not consider shorter time window because activity/power drops quickly beyond Β-band based on the power spectrum (see Materials and methods).

Reviewer #3:

[] The findings are of some interest but there are some major concerns. First, the discussion of previous work is rather limited and does not cite many related studies that have characterized the timing of face processing in the FFA and OFA. It has been known for a while that the OFA responds an earlier latency than the FFA (e.g., Liu et al., 2002), and that certain stimulus manipulations, such as face inversion and contrast reversal, lead to delayed responses to faces (Bentin et al., 1996; Rossion et al., 2000; Rossion et al., 2012). Previous fMRI work has shown that difficult to perceive Mooney faces can lead to response delays on the order of several seconds (McKeeff and Tong, 2007). More recent techniques have allowed research groups to provide more refined estimates of the timing of neural responses, such as the fusion of fMRI-MEG analyzed using representational similarity analysis (e.g., Cichy et al., 2014). Periodic visual stimulation has also been used to characterize the timing of neural responses obtained with EEG/MEG by several research groups (e.g., Rossion et al., 2012, 2014; Norcia et al., 2015), and this approach has been successfully applied to characterize top-down effects of feedback during face processing (e.g., Baldauf and Desimone, 2014). The empirical and conceptual advances made in the current study need to be more clearly articulated with respect to previous work, and a clear argument for the specific contributions of this study is needed.

We appreciate and agree with this suggestion. The dynamics of face induced neural activation in FFA and OFA has been studied for a long time with various techniques. However, previous results are inconsistent and individually often lack either the spatial (e.g., sensor level EEG/MEG analysis) or temporal precision (e.g., fMRI data). Our results with combined fMRI and MEG measures, provide detailed and novel timing information of the core face network. For example, the relatively large temporal gap between the right anterior and posterior FFA was not reported in previous studies. Furthermore, our results showed that the temporal relationships between OFA and FFA are dependent on the internal facial features as well the context of visual input, which helps to understand how bottom-up and top-down processing together contribute to face perception.

Many previous studies used the N170/M170 component as the index of face processing in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex, however, the delayed N170/M170 response caused by certain stimulus manipulations (eg: face inversion, Mooney transformation) represents a relatively crude measure of face processing because the difficulty in attributing the sources of the delay. On the other hand, fMRI measures alone showing delayed FFA response to Mooney faces that was initially not recognized as faces simply reflect the time it took subjects to recognize difficult Mooney faces, rather than the real-time dynamics of Mooney face processing. In contrast, our results showed that when the face features were confounded with other shadows, the top-down rpFFA to rOFA projection became more dominated.

In the revised manuscript, we discussed the different techniques used to investigate the timing of face responses and the top-down modulation in face processing reported in previous studies (Discussion section).

Another concern is that the conclusions of the study rest on the data from a single experiment, and further investigation of the putative effects of top-down feedback and predictive coding are not provided. Reproducibility is a serious concern in many fields of science, especially psychology and also neuroscience. A follow-up experiment that both replicates and extends the current findings would help strengthen the study. The reported effects pass statistical significance but not by a large margin. Moreover, there can be concerns that MEG data varies considerably across participants and can lead to heterogeneity of variance, especially across time points. Shuffling of the data with randomized labels would provide a more rigorous approach to statistical analysis.

We thank the editor and reviewer for pushing us to perform a follow-up experiment. We did not just one but three follow-up experiments (one replication and two extensions), which indeed replicated and significantly extended the findings reported in the original version.

We collected more data for Experiment 2 (normal unfamiliar face vs Mooney face) to confirm the previous results and performed two additional experiments to extend our findings.

The replication data and the new experiments are reported in the revised manuscript.

Replication: we collected data from 15 additional subjects using normal faces and Mooney faces. The results were consistent with previous ones with enhanced statistical power (see Results).

Extension 1: To further study the role of internal (eyes, nose, mouth) versus external (hair, chin, face outline) face features, we presented distorted face images (explicit internal facial features available but spatially misarranged without changing face contour) to subjects and analyzed data as before. Consistent with our hypothesis, the clear face components (even though misarranged) evoked strong resonses in rOFA, without clear evidence of a late signal corresponding to prediction error, indicating that spatial configuration of internal face features was not a prominent part of the prediction error from rFFA to rOFA. In this case, the processing sequence for the distorted faces would be similar to that elicited by normal face.

Extension 2: In a new experiment, we also investigated the role of context in face processing by presenting three types of stimuli to subjects: (i) images of highly degraded faces with contextual body toàn thân toàn thân cues which imply the presence of faces, (ii) images of degraded faces and body toàn thân toàn thân cues arranged in an incorrect configuration and thus do not imply the presence of faces, (iii) images of objects. Results showed that rOFA, rpFFA and raFFA are activated almost simultaneously a late stage, implying a parallel contextual modulation of the core faceprocessing network. This result further emphasize the importance of internal face features in driving the sequential OFA to FFA processing, and help our understanding of the dynamics of contextual modulation in face perception.

As described in #4 above, we collected data from additional 15 subjects for the Mooney face experiment (normal unfamiliar faces vs. Mooney faces). Combined with previous data, nonparametric permutation tests were performed to check the significance level of observed time difference between rOFA and rpFFA. The results are consistent with previous ones with enhanced statistical power (see Results).

Reviewer #4:

[] A point that is lacking significantly is the role of pSTS. We know pSTS is mostly involved in the analysis of facial muscle articulations (also called action units, AUs) and the interpretation of facial expressions and emotion, see Srinivasan et al., 2022, and Martinez, 2022. Also relevant is the role of low-level image features (Weibert et al., 2022), which is also missing from the Discussion; and, the role of color perception (Yip and Sinha, 2002; Benitez-Quiroz et al., 2022).

The temporal responses of bilateral pSTS are broader (multi-peaked) and showed lower signal-to-noise than the ventral face-selective areas (Figure 2 and Figure 2figure supplement 1). To increase our confidence about the pSTS time course, we analyzed the temporal responses of bilateral pSTS evoked by normal faces based on the additional data (Experiment 2), and the time courses basically remained the same as the previous ones (regardless of the task and face familiarity). We have added more discussion about the role of pSTS and its dynamics, especially in relation to the processing of facial expression, muscle articulations and motion.

We also thank the reviewer for reminding us about the role of low-level features including color, and have added more discussion about their role in face processing.

Another point that needs further discussion is the role of internal versus external face features (Sinha et al., 2006), and context (Sinha, Science 2004; Martinez, 2022).

These discussions are essential to frame the results of the present paper within existing models of face perception. With appropriate changes, this could be a strong paper.

We agree that it is important to understand the role of internal versus external face features. Since we were going to obtain more experimental data during the revision, we made the efforts to performed additional MEG experiments to specifically investigate the role of internal versus external face features and context (see response to editors #4). We have also added more discussion about them.

://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.48764.sa2

Reply

3

0

Chia sẻ

Share Link Tải Top-down processing Neuroscience miễn phí

Bạn vừa tìm hiểu thêm Post Với Một số hướng dẫn một cách rõ ràng hơn về Review Top-down processing Neuroscience tiên tiến và phát triển và tăng trưởng nhất Share Link Down Top-down processing Neuroscience Free.

Hỏi đáp vướng mắc về Top-down processing Neuroscience

Nếu sau khi đọc nội dung nội dung bài viết Top-down processing Neuroscience vẫn chưa hiểu thì hoàn toàn hoàn toàn có thể lại Comment ở cuối bài để Tác giả lý giải và hướng dẫn lại nha

#Topdown #processing #Neuroscience

Clip Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 ?

Bạn vừa Read nội dung bài viết Với Một số hướng dẫn một cách rõ ràng hơn về Clip Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 tiên tiến và phát triển nhất

Chia Sẻ Link Download Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 miễn phí

Bạn đang tìm một số trong những Share Link Cập nhật Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 miễn phí.

Hỏi đáp vướng mắc về Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022

Nếu sau khi đọc nội dung bài viết Top-down processing Neuroscience 2022 vẫn chưa hiểu thì hoàn toàn có thể lại Comment ở cuối bài để Tác giả lý giải và hướng dẫn lại nha
#Topdown #processing #Neuroscience

Phone Number

Share
Published by
Phone Number

Recent Posts

Tra Cứu MST KHƯƠNG VĂN THUẤN Mã Số Thuế của Công TY DN

Tra Cứu Mã Số Thuế MST KHƯƠNG VĂN THUẤN Của Ai, Công Ty Doanh Nghiệp…

2 years ago

[Hỏi – Đáp] Cuộc gọi từ Số điện thoại 0983996665 hoặc 098 3996665 là của ai là của ai ?

Các bạn cho mình hỏi với tự nhiên trong ĐT mình gần đây có Sim…

2 years ago

Nhận định về cái đẹp trong cuộc sống Chi tiết Chi tiết

Thủ Thuật về Nhận định về nét trẻ trung trong môi trường tự nhiên vạn…

2 years ago

Hướng Dẫn dooshku là gì – Nghĩa của từ dooshku -Thủ Thuật Mới 2022

Thủ Thuật về dooshku là gì - Nghĩa của từ dooshku -Thủ Thuật Mới 2022…

2 years ago

Tìm 4 số hạng liên tiếp của một cấp số cộng có tổng bằng 20 và tích bằng 384 2022 Mới nhất

Kinh Nghiệm Hướng dẫn Tìm 4 số hạng liên tục của một cấp số cộng…

2 years ago

Mẹo Em hãy cho biết nếu đèn huỳnh quang không có lớp bột huỳnh quang thì đèn có sáng không vì sao Mới nhất

Mẹo Hướng dẫn Em hãy cho biết thêm thêm nếu đèn huỳnh quang không còn…

2 years ago